Prospective Elementary Teachers' Conceptions of Rational Numbers

Dina Tirosh, School of Education, Tel-Aviv University, Israel

Efraim Fischbein, School of Education, Tel-Aviv University, Israel

Anna O. Graeber, College of Education, University of Maryland, USA

James W. Wilson, College of Education, University of Georgia, USA

August 8, 1998


The United State-Israel Binational Science Foundation

This research was supported by the United States-Israel Binational Science Grant (#88-00213/1). Any opinions, findings and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this publication are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of this foundation.


Introduction

Mathematics educators involved in elementary teacher education programs often voice their dissatisfaction with students' content knowledge of and attitudes toward mathematics. Prospective elementary teachers' mathematical knowledge is frequently described as insufficient (Ball, 1990; Even 1990; Graeber, Tirosh, and Glover, 1989; Reys, 1974; Simon, 1993; Wheeler, 1983). It is also often reported that prospective teachers view mathematics as a static body of knowledge composed of facts, methods and rules understood by only a very small portion of the population, while the rest dislike mathematics and believe they are doomed to failure in it (Becker, 1986; Baxter, 1983; Bulmahn and Young, 1982, Rees and Barr, 1984; Steffe, 1990; Tirosh, 1990).

This gloomy picture sets a real challenge to those teacher educators who believe that students' ways of thinking and attitudes toward a subject are a main component in any form and level of instruction. Clearly, comprehensive knowledge of prospective teachers' conceptions of the specific mathematical topics included in their curricula is essential for instruction in teacher education in much the same way as knowledge about children's conceptions is crucial in school instruction. In fact, it makes no sense to base teacher education programs on the assumption that teachers are tabula rasa than to assume that students enter their classrooms void of a wide range of conceptions if mathematics (Cooney, 1994). Thus some major, related goals for mathematics educators are: (1) to analyze prospective elementary teachers' ways of thinking about particular mathematical topics, (2) to develop didactic approaches that take account of these conceptions while aiming to help prospective teachers extend their own mathematical conceptions of these topics, and encouraging them to expand their notion of mathematics as a human activity, and (3) raising prospective teachers' confidence in their ability to do mathematics. Fulfilling these goals is of special importance as today's prospective teachers are tomorrow's teachers, and teachers are key figures in bringing about educational improvement (NCTM, 1989; NCTM, 1991).

A major part of the curricula of elementary schools, in most countries, is devoted to non-negative rational numbers. There is a considerable body of research on students' conceptions of rational numbers (e.g., Behr, Harel, Post, and Lesh, 1992; Carpenter, Fennema, and Romberg, 1993; Greer, 1992). Many researchers indicate that children experience difficulties with rational numbers and several reasons for these difficulties has been suggested: (1) children do not have the same everyday experience in using rational numbers as they do with natural numbers (Greer, 1994), (2) many children find it difficult to accept a given fraction as a number and tend to view it as two whole numbers (Hart, 1981; Kerslake, 1986), and (3) children often incorrectly attribute observed properties of operations with natural numbers to those with rational numbers (Bell, Fischbein and Greer, 1984; Hiebert and Wearne, 1986; Nesher, 1988; Owens, 1987; Sowder, 1988). Another source of children's confusion is the need to recognize and work with various interpretations of, and notations for, rational numbers (e.g., part-to-whole comparison, decimal, ratio, operator, indicated division, and measure of continuous and discrete quantities; Behr, Lesh, Post, and Silver, 1983; Freudenthal, 1983; Greer, 1992). In the light of these findings, it is notable that national and international studies consistently show that non-negative rational numbers are a stumbling block for many students (Carpenter, Corbitt, Kepner, Lindquist, and Reys, 1981; Carpenter, Lindquist, Brown, Kouba, Silver, and Swafford, 1988; Hart, 1981; Hiebert, 1988; Kieren, 1988; Travers and Westbury, 1990).

Given the central role of rational numbers in school mathematics, students' widely acknowledged difficulties in learning this topic, and the crucial role that teachers' subject matter knowledge of particular school topics plays in the instructional process (Ball, 1988; Leinhardt, Putnam, Stein and Baxter, 1991), it is somewhat surprising that only a few researchers have begun to study the knowledge base prospective teachers bring to the instruction of rational numbers. These researchers investigated the correctness and justifications of prospective elementary teachers' knowledge of division by fractions (Ball, 1990; Simon, 1993; Wheeler, 1983), their performance in solving multiplication and division word problems involving fractions (Greer and Mangan, 1986; Graeber and Tirosh, 1988) and their beliefs about multiplication and division (Tirosh and Graeber, 1990a). It has been shown that prospective elementary teachers' knowledge of rational numbers is procedural and sparsely connected. Although many prospective teachers could do calculation with rational numbers, they had significant difficulties in solving multiplication and division word problems, and their beliefs about the operations of multiplication and division were based solely on operations with natural numbers, and not adjusted top operations with rational numbers (Ball, 1990; Simon, 1993).

Each of these studies on prospective teachers' mathematical knowledge of rational numbers related to one or two specific aspects of this knowledge. In this chapter we shall describe and discuss a project one of whose main goals was to develop a conceptual framework for analyzing prospective teachers' mathematical knowledge of rational numbers. The Conceptual Adjustments in Progressing from Whole to Non-negative Rational Numbers (CAPWN) project is funded by the United States-Israel Binational Science Foundation, and four principal investigators are involved in it: James Wilson, from the University of Georgia, Anna Graeber, from the University of Maryland, and Efraim Fischbein and Dina Tirosh, from Tel-Aviv University. The project takes place in Israel and is currently in its third year.

In what follows, we shall present the major goals of our project, the conceptual framework that was used to analyze prospective teachers' mathematical knowledge of rational numbers, and some of the main findings. Then we shall provide an example of one situation taken from sessions with a class of prospective teachers that was particularly illuminating in terms of gaining a better grasp of the processes that take place when prospective teachers amend their mathematical content knowledge and revise their conceptions of the nature of mathematical activities, and of themselves as learners of mathematics.

The Main Goals of the CAPWM Project

The four main aims of this project are:

1. To understand children's and prospective elementary teachers' conceptions of rational numbers.

2. To develop didactic approaches aimed at helping prospective teachers extend their own mathematical conceptions of rational numbers and their knowledge about children's ways of thinking about them.

3. To involve prospective teachers in activities that encourage them to expand their notion of mathematics as a human activity.

4. To help prospective teachers decrease their mathematics anxiety and raise their confidence in their mathematics ability.

We shall deal, in this chapter, only with the part of the research that refers to prospective teachers. Let us start with describing the conceptual perspective we used to analyze prospective teachers' mathematical knowledge.

A Conceptual Framework for Analyzing Learners' Mathematical Knowledge

The conceptual framework for analyzing children' and adults' content knowledge of specific mathematical topics used in our project is based on the assumption that learners' mathematical knowledge is embedded in a set of connections among algorithmic, intuitive and formal dimensions of knowledge (Fischbein, 1983).

The algorithmic dimension is basically procedural in nature -- it consists of the rules and prescriptions of mathematics. This dimension also involves students' capability to explain the successive steps included in various, standard procedural operations.

The intuitive dimension comprises our ideas and beliefs about mathematical entities and the mental models we use for representing number concepts and operations with them. Intuitive knowledge is characterized as the type of knowledge that we tend to accept directly and confidently as being obvious, without feeling that it needs proof, and as having an imperative power, that is, it tends to eliminate other alternative representations, interpretations or solutions (Fischbein, 1987). Fischbein, Deri, Nello and Marino (1985) suggested, for example, that students' performance in solving mathematical word problems may be guided by primitive, behavioral models that do not apply to new concepts they encounter (e.g., repeated addition is a model for multiplication that works well for whole numbers but is too constraining for rational numbers).

The formal dimension includes the definitions of the concepts and of the operations, structures, and theorems relevant to a specific content domain. This type of knowledge is represented by axioms, definitions, theorems and their proofs. Both the formal and the algorithmic dimensions of knowledge can, for the student, become highly procedural and rote. Their vitality depends upon the student's constructing consistent connections among algorithms, intuitions and concepts.

Ideally, these dimensions of knowledge should cooperate in the processes of concept acquisition, understanding and problem solving. In reality, though, this is not always the case -- often there are serious inconsistencies between students' algorithmic, intuitive and formal knowledge. Such inconsistencies could be the source of common difficulties learners encounter in their mathematical activities, such as misconceptions, cognitive obstacles and inadequate usage of algorithms.

The three dimensions of knowledge are not discrete; they overlap considerably. In fact, any mathematical activity requires the use of all three of them. Nonetheless, it is useful to focus on them separately when the aim is to assess a student's mathematical knowledge with regard to a certain domain.

The following section, which describes prospective elementary teachers' knowledge of rational numbers, illustrates how the suggested framework is used to analyze learners' knowledge of rational numbers.

Prospective Elementary Schools Teachers' Mathematical Knowledge of Rational Numbers: Some Major Findings

Prospective elementary teachers' mathematical knowledge of rational numbers was assessed during the first and second years of the project. We shall briefly describe the methodology and some of the findings related to this part of the project.

 

Subjects

 

One-hundred-forty-seven elementary prospective teachers in their first year of teachers' college participated in this part of the study. Twenty-six of them were mathematics majors while the other 121 students had chosen a different main subject (biology, music, English, etc.).

 

 

The diagnostic questionnaire

 

All subjects were asked to complete a diagnostic questionnaire which examined their formal, algorithmic and intuitive understanding of rational numbers. The diagnostic questionnaire examined the following aspects of prospective teachers' knowledge of rational numbers:

a) The algorithm dimension: Ability to compute with rational numbers and to explain the successive steps of the standard algorithms used for operations with fractions and decimals. Other components examined, but not reported here, are related to transformation from fractions to decimals and vice versa, and undefined mathematical expressions involving fractions.

b) The formal dimension: Ability to define rational and irrational numbers, knowledge related to the hierarchy of several sets of numbers (natural numbers, integers, rational numbers, irrational numbers, real numbers), to the set membership of various numbers and to the density of the rational numbers, and familiarity with the commutative, associative and distributive laws.

c) The intuitive dimension: Capability to produce adequate intuitive models for representing number concepts and operations with them, and competency in evaluating the adequacy of statements related to arithmetic operations (e.g., "division always makes smaller", "multiplication always makes bigger").

Some examples of the various types of items included in the diagnostic questionnaire are provided in Appendix 1 and Appendix 2.

 

The interviews

 

Three phases of interviews were conducted at the first stage of the study. These interviews were conducted to further our understanding of the prospective teachers algorithmic, formal and intuitive dimensions of knowledge and their interconnections.

Phase 1: Algorithmic and intuitive understanding of rational numbers.

Twenty five prospective teachers who exhibited difficulties in solving multiplication and division problems had at least three 20-40 minute interviews.

Each interview consisted of two parts: The main part consisted of questions outlined specifically for each subject, based on her responses to the multiplication and division algorithmically-based problems. During the interview, each subject was asked to (1) perform drills which were similar to the ones she missed on the diagnostic questionnaire, and to explain her performance, and (2) to explain the rationale of the various algorithmic procedures. The second part included tasks presented to all interviewees. They were asked to determine the correctness of several statements related to the operations of multiplication and division.

A sample outline of an interview for one of the subjects is provided in Appendix 1.

Phase 2: Prospective teachers' formal and intuitive understanding of rational numbers.

Eighteen subjects who exhibited serious deficiencies in their formal understanding of rational numbers were interviewed. The interviews were semi-structured, that is, an "interview program" was outlined for all subjects, yet additional probes were made during interviews to better understand the subjects' conceptions. Each prospective teacher had at least three 20-45 minute interviews.

A full description of the interview program is provided in Appendix 2.

Phase 3: Prospective teachers' facility in representing rational numbers.

 

Thirty-two non-mathematics majors, constituting all the students in two of the classes involved in this study, were selected as participants in this phase of the interview. The participants were asked to:

- construct as many representations as possible of

 

- find the unit of a given representation (area, number line and set) of the stated fractional part

- identify critical features of representations of , and of

- indicate how they would respond to students who gave particular representations of (some of the representations were appropriate while others were not).

Each prospective teacher had at least 20-45 minute interviews. A sample of some of the tasks included in these interviews are provided in Appendix 3.

 

Results

 

A. Prospective teachers' algorithmic knowledge of rational numbers

Most prospective teachers were successful in performing addition, subtraction and multiplication with fractional numbers and with decimals. Prospective teachers who incorrectly solved these problems showed the same, incorrect response patterns as did school students on these tasks (e.g., adding numerators to obtain the numerator of the sum, and similarly, adding the denominators). The situation with respect to solving division problems with fractions and decimals was less satisfactory (see Table 1). The mathematics majors performed better than the non-mathematics majors in these computational tasks, but neither of the two groups performed satisfactorily. The most problematic items were those involving division of decimal numbers -- most of the mathematics-major prospective teachers who solved these items correctly first wrote the decimals in their fractional form and then solved these items. The most frequent incorrect responses to the decimal problems involved incorrect replacement of the decimal points, or a statement to the effect that this problem could not be performed, the dividend being smaller than the divisor. Incorrect responses to division in its fractional form included taking the inverse of the dividend instead of that of the divisor, and, in cases such as : 3, multiplying both the numerator and the denominator by 3.

Table 1

 

Sample of Responses to Decimal Division Computations

The problem

Mathematics majors

correct (%)

Others

correct (%)

0.8 : 0.2

0.2 : 0.8

0.2 : 3

0.25 : 0.6

8.25 : 4.5

93

86

86

64

57

62

69

43

29

12

The prospective teachers were generally unable to justify the successive steps of the standard algorithms of arithmetic operations with decimals and fractions. We shall refer, for instance, to prospective teachers' responses to the item which relate to the placement of the decimal point in multiplication of decimal numbers (see Appendix 1, problem 2). Although all participants knew that the student's answer was correct, only one was able to justify the place of the decimal point in the product. The others said that they simply "do it" this way. They were unable to call upon their knowledge of fractions or their knowledge of multiplication with whole numbers as possible sources for answers. In fact, they were quite surprised there should be a need to explain why certain algorithmic steps are performed the way they are. During the interviews, when they were encouraged to attempt to inquire why, when multiplying decimal numbers, the decimal point is put, at the end of the process, at a certain place, the vast majority of them expressed beliefs to the effect that "If you don't know how to explain it you won't be able to find it by yourself, no matter how hard you think about it. Such knowledge could not be reached by us. Someone who knows the reasons has to tell us; then we shall remember them and tell them to our students".

With regard to division of decimals, the prospective teachers were presented with the following item:

A student solves the expression 5 : 0.8 in the following manner:

1st step

 

2nd step

 

3rd step

1. Explain steps 1 and 2.

2. What is the answer to the original division problem (5 : 0.8)?

Most prospective teachers knew that the student's answer is correct, yet only eight provided adequate explanations for steps 1 and 2. The most frequent, inappropriate answer was 0.625, as "we need to divide the answer by 10 in order to compensate for the previous multiplication of the dividend and the divisor by 10".

 

B. Prospective teachers' formal knowledge of rational numbers

Substantial differences were identified between the performance of the mathematics majors and that of the non-mathematics majors on each of the items included in this category. While the vast majority of the mathematics majors (92%) correctly defined rational and irrational numbers, only 23% of the other were able to do so; 81% of the mathematics majors and 25% of the non-mathematics majors drew an adequate Venn diagram to describe the relations between the natural numbers, the integers, the rational numbers, the irrational numbers and the real numbers; 92% of the mathematics majors correctly described the commutative law, while only 54% of the non-majors did so; 85% of the mathematics majors gave a correct description of the distributive law while only 12% of the non-majors did so; and finally, 77% of the mathematics majors were able to state the associative law while only 3% of the non-mathematics majors did so.

By and large, the mathematics-major prospective teachers showed adequate formal knowledge of rational numbers. The non-major mathematics prospective teachers' formal knowledge, however, was insufficient, incomplete, and dull.

In the second year of this study, prospective teachers' knowledge of two additional components related to the formal dimension were explored among 37 non-mathematics major prospective teachers: identification of the set membership of various numbers, and the density of the rational numbers. Prospective teachers were asked to determine the set membership of 10 numerical expressions. For instance, they had to determine if -3 is a natural number, an integer, a rational number, an irrational number, and/or a real number. Their performance on this item was very poor. Only 8% and 22% of the prospective teachers, respectively, knew that 0 and 0.251 are rational numbers. Twenty-four percent argued that 2/0 is an integer. Confusion reigned concerning the notion of real numbers -- the majority of the prospective teachers mistakenly identified "real numbers" with "positive numbers", and consequently argued that all the given numerical expressions, except and -3, were real numbers. Others argued that real numbers were "nice numbers", namely, that 0.42, , 0.251, , and -3 were real numbers, while 0.121221222, ¸, and 0 were not.

Only few prospective teachers were aware of the density of rational numbers: 24% knew that between and there are infinite amounts of numbers. In fact, 43% claimed that there are no numbers between and , and 30% claimed that is the successor of . With regard to decimals, the results were somewhat better. For instance, 40% knew that between 0.23 and 0.24 there is an infinity of decimals and could even present some of them.

C. Prospective teachers' intuitive knowledge of rational numbers

Many prospective teachers held primitive beliefs concerning the results of multiplication and division. These primitive beliefs were widespread both among those who did and those who did not major in mathematics. In fact, 60% of the prospective teachers incorrectly argued that "multiplication always makes bigger", and 51% incorrectly argued that "division always makes smaller".

As expected, the vast majority of the prospective teachers used common ways of illustrating fractions, including graphic representations (either disks or rectangular regions), concrete representations (e.g., apples, chocolate bars) or verbal representations (i.e., word problems) to construct representations of fractions and operations with fractions. Table 2 shows that most prospective teachers drew appropriate representations of one-third, in which the unit was evident, the three parts appeared to be reasonably equal and one of the three parts was designated. The representations of operations involving fractions were much more difficult tasks for the prospective teachers. Only few constructed appropriate representations of multiplication and division expressions involving fractions. No substantial differences were found between mathematics major and non-mathematics major prospective teachers' intuitive knowledge of rational numbers.

Most prospective teachers lacked the ability to construct appropriate representations of fractions and operations with rational numbers.

 

Table 2

Prospective Teachers' Representations for Fractions and Operations with Fractions (* )

 Appropriate Representations  Inappropriate Representations
   Verbal  Graphic  Concrete  Verbal  Graphic  Concrete

 

 16

 54

 23

 5

 9

 0

 11:6

 42

 0

 25

 8

 16

 0

6:11 

41

1

23

10

8

0

 

 22

12

11

3

7

1

 

 14

16

4

17

16

4

 

 12

6

5

8

8

10

 

 7

5

5

18

19

7

 

 3

2

1

15

23

9

 

 1

0

1

17

12

5

 

(*) some of the prospective teachers provided more than one representation for the given expression

In order to better understand the prospective teachers' difficulties with these items, we conducted intensive individual interviews with the subjects. They were asked, among other things, to construct as many different representations as possible for and and to comment on the appropriateness of given representations (area, set, number line, and ratio) of and . Some of the appropriate and inappropriate representations that were used in these interviews were taken from the prospective teachers' responses to the diagnostic questionnaires. Other representations used during this interview were adapted from the research literature on children's conceptions of mathematical operations (see Appendix 3, item 2 for a description of some of the representations used during this phase of the interview).

It was found that prospective teachers rely primarily on the part-whole interpretation of fractions. Most of them drew appropriate area models for and . Despite the instruction to give as many representations as possible, most prospective teachers gave only one, the area model representation of these fractions. When asked to give alternative models, most prospective teachers simply changed the geometrical shape used in the area models. Some referred to concrete objects such as watermelons or chocolate bars. Very few constructed set models, and no number line or ratio models were produced.

The predominance of the part-whole interpretation and of the area model of fractions seriously limited the subjects' ability to represent the fraction . Only two prospective teachers drew successful models of ; both were area models. The approach and comments of those who were unsuccessful revealed some of the difficulties with representing improper fractions. A typical approach was to draw a rectangle, portion it into three parts, and then append two more same-sized parts. This approach resulted in a rectangle partitioned into five equal sections. Many then looked at their drawing and evaluated it as incorrect, making comments such as "You can't illustrate out of the whole".

When the prospective teachers were asked to comment on given number line and set model representations of and , the predominance of the area model was much in evidence. The prospective teachers tended to reject a set model of in which one of three triangles with unequal areas was marked. They justified this response with the argument that "the areas of the three figures included in the set are not equal, and thus one triangle can't represent on-third." In commenting on the appropriateness of number line models, many prospective teachers readily accepted a number line on which points 0, 1, 2, and 3 were labeled and the segment between zero and one was shaded, as a model of one-third. They justified this by saying that the segment from 0 to 1 was one of the three parts (0 to 1, 1 to 2, and 2 to 3). None of them mentioned that on the number line the unit is generally defined by the distance from zero to one and that one-third is associated with a point situated one-third of the way from 0 to 1.

The representation of was a more difficult task for the prospective teachers than that of , or even . Representations were considered appropriate if 1) the unit for the two addends were the same and evident, 2) one-half was distinguished; one-third was distinguished, and 3) the union of the two parts was identified by some means other than use of a plus sign between two disjoint parts. Only about a third of the prospective teachers gave representations that met these criteria, and all of these were area models. Most subjects whose responses were classified as inappropriate did not recognize the need to use the same unit for the two addends.

The predominance of the part-whole interpretation and of the area model interferes with many other representations of rational numbers. This restricted perception directed prospective teachers towards a narrow view of rational numbers. Clearly, it is important to promote prospective teachers' familiarity with various representations of rational numbers, as different representations emphasize different aspects of these numbers.

D. Interconnections between algorithmic, formal and intuitive knowledge

In the course of analyzing the data from the diagnostic test and from the various interview sessions, we became more and more aware of apparent contradictions between prospective teachers' algorithmic performance, their formal knowledge and their intuitive beliefs about the arithmetic operations. The most prevalent inconsistencies became apparent when comparing computational skills with their intuitive knowledge about the mathematical operations. For instance, all the subjects who argued that division always makes smaller(about a half of the prospective teachers), correctly computed at least some of the division problems that resulted in a quotient which was greater than the dividend.

The various interview sessions gave insight into possible sources of these inconsistencies. we shall not be able to thoroughly discuss issues related to these inconsistencies within the framework of this paper. The reader could refer to some other papers that deal with such discrepancies (Ball, 1990; Simon, 1993; Tirosh and Graeber, 1990a; Tirosh and Graeber, 1990b).

 

Summary

 

Generally speaking, the prospective teachers' mathematical knowledge was found to be rigid and segmented. Most of them operated almost totally with rigid algorithms and procedural knowledge about rational numbers concepts, were generally unable to justify the successive steps of the algorithms, held primitive models of the operations, could not produce adequate representations of rational number concepts or operations with rational numbers, and lacked a representation of mathematics as an organized and structured body of knowledge. Mathematics for most of them was a mere collection of computational techniques not well mastered, unjustified formally, indeed often even intuitively.

The mathematics-major prospective teachers' performance on most algorithmic and formal items somewhat exceeded that of the non-majors. These differences in performance may be explained by two main factors: (1) the mathematical background of mathematics majors is stronger than that of non-mathematics majors (the former took more mathematics in high school), and (2) in general, mathematics majors exhibited more positive attitudes toward mathematics and towards their ability to study it than did non-mathematics majors. However, as noted previously, the mathematics knowledge of both these groups of prospective teachers was insufficient, rigid and segmented.

We have tried to explain the reasons for this lack of knowledge on the part of the prospective teachers. The most probable explanation we can come up with is that the curricula in the teachers' colleges do not, in general, sufficiently emphasize the mathematical topics taught in elementary school class. Prospective teachers learned these notions in their childhood, when the accent may have been (excessively at times) on concrete representations and particular solving techniques. Consequently, they will only have accumulated fragmentary knowledge without an integrating perspective, and obscured behavior with rational numbers, therefore, is often limited to recalling and applying memorized symbol manipulation rules.

This explanation is supported by the fact that the difficulties our subjects experienced with rational numbers are often similar to the ones children are known to confront. This may also indicate that the same mechanisms of reasoning are at the source of both children and adults' limited understanding of rational numbers. It seemed that students' and prospective teachers' conceptions of numbers were almost entirely based on natural numbers. This allowed them to partially operate on the algorithmic level (i.e., using memorized algorithms to perform given, computational expressions), but blocked their ability to cope with tasks that demanded adequate formal and/or intuitive knowledge of rational numbers.

The main conclusion we draw from this part of the research is that elementary arithmetic topics should be included in the curriculum of the teachers' colleges, but considered from a different perspective. Such a curriculum should attempt to expand prospective teachers' formal, algorithmic and intuitive understanding of the concepts and processes included in the elementary school curricula and the interactions among them. Prospective teachers should be exposed to historical perspectives on the concepts at issue and to the course of their evolution. They should become acquainted with the research findings that are relevant to the understanding of the learning processes of these concepts. They should be encouraged to adopt the view that the mistakes they and their future students make may stem from a systematic line of thinking and that some of them are to be regarded as conceptual obstacles whose identification and treatment is essential to the process of learning these tasks. Prospective teachers should be encouraged to reflect on their own thinking and to analyze that of their students.

In respect to rational numbers, it is essential that the curricula for elementary school teachers attempt to increase their familiarity with common representations of and with them; boost their understanding of the similarities and differences of these representations, and their ability to use them flexibly. Such curricula should also encourage prospective teachers to engage in activities aimed at improving their computation skills and their understanding of the rationale behind the algorithms. It is also essential that prospective teachers become acquainted with the definitions of the set of real numbers and its subsets, and develop a clear idea of the interrelations between the various sets of numbers. Such knowledge, on the part of the teacher, seems necessary for allowing him/her to introduce rational number concepts in a manner that will help students develop strong conceptual underpinnings of rational numbers and of the processes underlying their operations.

In the course of the study we observed that the prospective teachers experienced significant difficulty when reflecting on their own thinking and analyzing the possible sources of their own mistakes. The prospective teachers were generally unable to list students' common ways of thinking about rational numbers. It was decided that the curricula for prospective teachers should reflect the accumulating knowledge related to children's understanding of rational numbers and to their difficulties in grasping the pertinent concepts and processes. More specifically, the various stumbling blocks related to the transition from natural to rational numbers -- difficulties that have been discussed quite extensively in the research literature during the past two decades -- have to be addressed by the curricula for elementary school teachers (e.g., the natural tendency to transfer the constraints of operations with natural numbers [such as 'the dividend is never smaller than the divisor'] to operations with rational numbers).

In light of these observations, we concluded that a main goal of teacher education should be to develop prospective teachers' mathematical and pedagogical content knowledge of rational numbers. Such a demanding task necessitates (1) more comprehensive knowledge about prospective teachers' mathematical and pedagogical content knowledge of rational numbers, and (2) the development of a special course for prospective elementary teachers, which attempts to improve prospective teachers' algorithmic, formal and intuitive knowledge of rational numbers, as well as increasing their knowledge about students' conceptions and ways of thinking about rational numbers and their possible sources. The second, (current) stage of our study was devoted to the elaboration and the experimental teaching of a prototype module on rational numbers.

 

Final Remark on the conceptual framework for analyzing learners' mathematical knowledge

We have argued that the conceptual framework that was used in our study is applicable not only to the domain of the rational numbers, but to other mathematical domains as well. This, of course, deserves further research.

Not less important is the need to learn more about the three dimensions of knowledge described in this model. Questions related to the development of each of these dimensions of knowledge, to the nature of the interconnections, and to implications of this model on the development of didactic materials to be used in elementary teacher education were not given the attention they deserve in this paper. We hope to elaborate more on this issue during the symposium.

 

References

Arcavi, A., Tirosh, D., & Nachmias, R. (1989). The effects of exploring a new representation on prospective mathematics teachers' conception of functions. In S. Vinner (Ed.). Proceedings of the Second International Jerusalem Convention on Education: Science and Mathematics Education -- Interaction between Research and Practice (pp. 269-277). Israel: University of Jerusalem.

Ball, D. (1988). Unlearning to teach mathematics. For the Learning of Mathematics, 8, 40-48.

Ball, D. (1990). Prospective elementary and secondary teachers' understanding of division. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 21, 132-144.

Baxter, J. (1983). Status and trends in mathematics teacher education. In M. Zweng, T. Green, J. Kilpatrick, H. Pollak, & M. Suydam (Eds.), Proceedings of the Fourth International Congress on Mathematics Education (pp. 90-92). Berkeley, CA: Bikhauser.

Becker, J.R. (1986). Mathematics attitudes of elementary education majors. Arithmetic Teacher, 33, 50-52.

Behr, M., Harel, G., Post, T., & Lesh, L. (1992). Rational numbers, ratio and proportion. In D.A. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 296-333). New York: Macmillan

Behr, M., Lesh, R., Post, T., & Silver, E.A. (1983). Rational number concepts. In R. Lesh & M. Landau (Eds.), Acquisition of mathematical concepts and processes (pp. 91-126). New York: Academic Press.

Bell, A., Fischbein, E., & Greer, B. (1984). Choice of operation in verbal arithmetic problems: The effects of number size, problem structure and content. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 12, 399-420.

Borasi, R. (1987). An inquiry into the nature of mathematical definitions. Unpublished manuscript.

Bulmahn, B., & Young, D.M. (1982). On the transmission of mathematics anxiety. Arithmetic Teacher, 30, 55-56.

Carpenter, T.C., Corbitt, M.K., Kepner, H.S., Lindquist, M.N., & Reys, R.E. (1981). Decimals: Results and implications from national assessment. Arithmetic Teacher, 28, 34-37.

Carpenter, T.C., Corbitt, M.K., Reys, R.E., & Wilson, J. (1976). Notes from national assessment: Addition and multiplication with fractions. Arithmetic Teacher, 23, 137-141.

Carpenter, T.C., Fenemma, E., & Romberg, T. (Eds.) (1993). Rational numbers: An integration of research. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Carpenter, T.C., Lindquist, M.M., Brown, C.A., Kouba, V.L., Silver, E.A. & Swafford, J.O. (1988). Results of the fourth NAEP assessment of mathematics: Trends and conclusions. Arithmetic Teachers, 36, 38-41.

Confery, J. (1990). What constructivism implies for teaching. In R.B. Davis, C.A. Maher, & N. Noddings (Eds.), Constructivist views on teaching and learning mathematics (Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, Monograph No. 4, pp. 107-124). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

Cooney, T. (1994). Teacher education as an exercise in adaptation. In D. B. Achele & A. F. Coxford (Eds.), Professional Development for teachers of Matheamatics (pp. 9-22). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

D'Ambrosio, B., & Campos, T.M.M. (1992). Pre-service teachers' representations of children's understanding of mathematical concepts: Conflicts and conflict resolution. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 23, 213-230.

Even, R. (1990). Subject matter knowledge for teaching and the case of functions. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 21, 521-544.

Even, R., & Markovitz, Z. (1991). Teachers' pedagogical knowledge: The case of functions. In F. Furinghetti (Ed.), Proceedings of the Fifteenth International Conference for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (Vol. 22, pp. 40-47). Assisi, Italy.

Even, R., & Tirosh D. (1995). Subject-matter knowledge and knowledge about students as sourches of teacher presentations of the subject-matter. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 29, 1-20.

Fenemma, E., Carpenter, T., & Peterson, P.L. (1989). Learning mathematics with understanding; Cognitively guided instruction. In J.E. Brophy (Ed.), Advances in research in teaching: Vol. I. Teaching for meaningful understanding and self-regulated learning (pp. 195-221). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

Fischbein, E. (1987). Intuition in science and mathematics. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Reidel.

Fischbein, E., Deri, M., Nello, M. & Marino, M. (1985). The role of implicit models in solving problems in multiplication and division. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 16, 3-17.

Freudenthal, H. (1983). Didactical phenomenology of mathematical structures. Boston: D. Reidel.

Graeber, A., & Tirosh, D. (1988). Multiplication and division involving decimals: Preservice teachers' performance and beliefs. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 7, 263-280.

Graeber, A., Tirosh, D., & Glover, R. (1989). Preservice teachers' misconceptions in solving verbal problems in multiplication and division. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 20, 95-102.

Greer, B. (1992). Multiplication and division as models of situations. In D.A. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (276-295), New York: Macmillan.

Greer, B. (1994). Mathematics: Topics of instruction (rational numbers). In H. Torsten, & N. Postlethwaite (Eds.), International encyclopedia of education (second edition, 3675-3676). London: Pergamon Press.

Greer, B., & Mangan, C. (1986). Choice of operations: From 10- years-old to student teachers. Proceedings of the Tenth International Conference for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (pp. 25-30). London, England.

Hart, K.(Ed.) (1981). Childrens' understanding of mathematics: 11-16. London: Murray.

Herskowitz, R., Vinner, S., & Bruckheimer, M. (1978). The challenge in mistakes (in Hebrew). Sevavim, 17, 1-11.

Hiebert, J. (1988). A theory of developing competence with written mathematical symbols. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 19, 333-355.

Hiebert, J., & Wearne, D. (1986). Procedures over concepts: The acquisition of decimal number knowledge. In J. Hiebert (Ed.), Conceptual and procedural knowledge: The case of mathematics (pp. 258-268). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Kerslake, D. (1986). Fractions: Children's strategies and errors. Windsor, England: Nfer-Nelson.

Kieren, T. E. (1988). Personal knowledge of rational numbers: Its intuitive and formal development. In J. Hiebert & M. Behr (Eds.), Number concepts and operations in the middle grades (pp. 162-181). Reston, VA: NCTM; Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Kline, M. (1980). Mathematics, the loss of certainty. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Leinhardt, G., Putman, R.T., Stein, M.K., & Baxter, J. (1991). Where subject knowledge matters. In J.E. Brophy (Ed.), Advances in research in teaching (Vol. II, pp. 87-113). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

Maher, C.A., & Alston, A. (1990). Teacher development in mathematics in a constructivist framework. In R.B. Davis, C., A. Maher, & N. Noddings (Eds.), Constructivist views on teaching and learning mathematics (Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, Monograph No. 4, pp. 147-166). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

Meyerson, L. (1976). Mathematical mistakes. Mathematics Teacher, 71, 737-738.

Mochon, S. (1993). When can you meaningfully add rates, ratios and fractions? For the Learning of Mathematics, 13, 16-21.

National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (1989). Curriculum and evaluation standards for school mathematics. Reston, VA: NCTM.

National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (1991). Professional standards for teaching mathematics. Reston, VA: NCTM.

Nesher, P. (1988). Multiplicative school word problems: Theoretical approaches and empirical findings. In J. Hiebert & M. Behr (Eds.), Number concepts and operations in the middle grades (pp. 19-40). Reston, VA: NCTM; Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Owens, D.T. (1987). Decimal multiplication in grade seven. In J.C. Bergeron, N. Herscovics, & C. Kieren (Eds.), Proceedings of the Eleventh International Conference for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (Vol. 2, pp. 423-429). Montreal, Canada.

Rees, R., & Barr, G. (1984). Diagnosis and prescription in the classroom: Some common maths problems. London: Harper & Row.

Reys, R.E. (1974). Division by zero: An area of needed research. Arithmetic teacher, 21, 153-157.

Simon, M. (1993). Prospective elementary teachers' knowledge of division. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 24, 233-254.

Sowder, L. (1988). Children's solutions of word problem. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 7, 227-238.

Steffe, L. (1990). On the knowledge of mathematics teachers. In R.B. Davis, C.A. Maher, & N. Noddings (Eds.), Constructivist views on teaching and learning mathematics (Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, Monograph No. 4, pp. 167-186). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

Steffe, L. (1991). The constructivist teaching experiment: Illustrations and implications. In E. von Glaserfeld (Ed.), Radical constructivism in mathematics education (pp. 177-194). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer.

Tirosh, D. (1990). Improving prospective early childhood teachers' content knowledge and attitudes toward mathematics. In L.P. Steffe & T. Wood (Eds.), Transforming children's mathematics education: International perspectives. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Tirosh, D. (1993). Teachers' understanding of undefined mathematical expressions (in Spanish), Substratum, 1, 61-86.

Tirosh, D., & Graeber, A. (1990a). Inconsistencies in preservice elementary teachers' beliefs about multiplication and division. Focus on Learning Problems in Mathematics, 20, 95-102,

Tirosh, D., & Graeber, A. (1990b). Evoking cognitive conflict to explore preservice teachers' thinking about division. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 21, 98-108.

Travers, K.J., & Westbury, I. (1990). The IEA study of mathematics. Oxford: Pergamon Press.

Watson, J. M., Collis, K. F., & Campell, K. J. (1995). Developmental structure in the understanding of common and decimal fractions. Focus on Learning Problems in Mathematics, 17, 1-24.

Wheeler, M.M. (1983). Much ado about nothing: Preservice elementary school teachers' concept of zero. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 14, 147-155.

Wood, T., Cobb, P., & Yachel, E. (1991). Reflections on learning and teaching mathematics in elementary school. In L.P. Steffe & J. Gale (Eds.), Constructivism in education. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.


Return